Document Type : Original Article
Authors
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Allameh Tabataba’i University, Tehran, IRAN
Abstract
Keywords
Introduction
Creating favorable conditions and providing opportunities for achieving a deeper and more lasting level of learning is one of the main goals and ideals of instructional technology. Taking advantage of scientific and instructional techniques, tools, and media, technologists and instructional designers seek to design and present the instructional content in a way that is more attractive and easier to understand for learners. Despite this, many technology-based instructions and instructional designs are not of enough effectiveness and efficiency. These types of instructions, rather than facilitating and accelerating learning for the learner, especially for his working memory as a learning bottleneck, induce load or cognitive density, thus slowing and even impeding the process of learning (1). A number of researchers and educational specialists believe that the difficulty of learning a subject arises from its cognitive load (2). The term cognitive load refers to the amount of load on short-term or working memory when processing data to encode that information to accommodate to the long-term memory. This mental effort for processing information is called cognitive load (3). The basic premise of cognitive load theory is that learners, when faced with new information, have a very limited working memory capacity for processing. According to this theory, if the mental burden of instructional content goes beyond the limited capacity of working memory, learning will be disrupted. If the number of elements to be processed simultaneously in the working memory is very high, too high memory capacity is required, and this leads to cognitive overload (4).
Cognitive load theory poses three kinds of cognitive load including intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive loads. Intrinsic cognitive load is determined by the interaction between the nature of the learning material and the learner’s level of expertise. Intrinsic cognitive load depends on the number of elements that should be processed simultaneously in working memory. Extraneous cognitive load is related to the processes that not only are not essential but also disrupt learning, and they should be changed through a series of educational interventions. The cause of extraneous cognitive load in multimedia material is providing poor problem-solving techniques, providing information resources that are distracting, unnecessary information-seeking tasks to perform and so on. Germane load, which is caused by cognitive activities that contribute to learning, has been introduced into the theory at a later stage to account for the learning-relevant demands on working memory (3).
Numerous researches have been done in the field of cognitive load theory related to the design of instructional content. A series of studies conducted by Mayer and his colleagues were related to the split-attention effect. It was found that instructions consisting of separate text and unlabelled diagrams were less effective than diagrams that contained labels that clearly connected text and diagram (5, 6). Van Merrienboer and de Croock also conducted a similar study with computer programming content. A generation (conventional) group was compared with a completion group in learning programming techniques. Results indicated superior learning by the completion group. When using a completion strategy, the presentation of new information and programming practice was linked to incomplete programs, and learners were only required to complete the partial solutions, whereas the generation strategy presented both model programs and generation assignments (7). Mayer et al. found that instructional multimedia designed according to multimedia principles lead to more learning compared to conventional methods (8). Kalyuga, Chandler, and Sweller used computer-based instructions in mechanical engineering to compare three different forms of textual explanations presented together with an animated diagram: written text, spoken text and written plus spoken text. The results demonstrated a multimedia redundancy effect. The spoken text group outperformed the written text plus spoken text group with a higher posttest score, a lower number of re-attempts at interactive exercises and a lower subjective rating of cognitive load. Subjective ratings of cognitive load indicated that presenting on-screen textual explanations of the diagram together with the same auditory explanations actually resulted in additional cognitive load (9). In Khalil et al.’s study, which was conducted within the framework of cognitive load theory with interactive anatomical images to achieve the instructional objectives, the researchers showed that with the increasing complexity of instructional materials, images imposed more cognitive load and mental effort, hence reducing learning (10). Paying attention to the human cognitive architecture and taking advantage of the principles of cognitive load theory, Kahol, Vankipuram, and Smith developed a simulator for laparoscopic surgery. Results showed that the experimental group outperformed the control group after eight instructional sessions (11). Mayer and colleagues (as cited in Sweller et al.) demonstrated that representations based on dual channels lowered the information load from the visual channel, reduced extraneous cognitive load, and thus, left more resources for germane cognitive processing (12). Mahbubi et al. showed that the observance of 14 principles of instructional design had a significant effect on reducing cognitive load in the experimental group compared to the control group and also reduced the extraneous cognitive load (13). Mosaramezani, Kanani, and Velayati found that reducing extraneous load and managing intrinsic cognitive load at the same time could increase students’ learning. In addition, managing intrinsic cognitive load and reducing extraneous cognitive load simultaneously increases students’ retention (14). Kyun, Kalyuga and Sweller showed the effect of reducing extraneous cognitive load on learning of English literature. Students with less knowledge in the worked-example group showed a better performance in problems in the learning stage, although significant effects were not observed in transfer tests, the worked-example group had a better performance compared to traditional problem solving group in the retention test (15). Nückles et al. showed that by increasing learners’ skills in journalism, external guidance would be an additional element, thus imposing the extraneous cognitive load. Accordingly, the gradual elimination of instructional guidance with increasing levels of expertise is effective in reducing the negative effects of negative support (16). Qiao et al. found that in sciences such as anatomy and pathology more attention should be paid to the extraneous cognitive load caused by false instructional design. They argued that overlooking this type of load in instructional content poses challenges that might hinder students’ learning (17). They also pointed out that cognitive load theory’s principles and procedures can be a useful framework for understanding the challenges and success related to the education of medical specialists. Zare, Salari and Sarikhani found that the use of cognitive load theory’s instructional strategies could reduce extraneous cognitive load and enhance learning in physiology courses (18). A review of the studies in the field of education and cognitive load shows that using cognitive load theory’s effects and strategies in the teaching-learning process can lead to more efficient instructional materials and, accordingly, effective learning outcomes. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of instructional multimedia based on reducing the extraneous cognitive load in English language learning among Nursing students.
Methods
The present study was quasi-experimental in nature and pretest-posttest with control group in design. The population involved all the undergraduate students of Nursing at Alborz University of Medical Sciences during the academic year 2015-2016. The students taking part in the study were selected based on a convenience sampling procedure and were, then, randomly assigned to either control (n = 17) or experimental group (n = 19). The inclusion criterion was the students’ consent to participation in the study.
Two instruments were employed in the study: instructional multimedia lessons and an English grammar test. The instructional multimedia lessons included two separate lessons designed and developed for the two groups in the study. One of the lessons was expected to induce high extraneous load due to the presence of the isolated elements effect (for the control group), and the other inducing low extraneous load due to the elimination of the isolated elements effect (for the experimental group). In terms of contents, the lessons were identical and consisted of English grammar instructional points.
The English grammar test included a 20-item multiple-choice test measuring the grammar points delivered via the instructional multimedia lessons. The test was used as both pretest and posttest; the order of the items across the two administrations was changed in order to minimize practice effect. Each correct item was given one point, adding up to the maximum score of 20. In terms of content validity, two English language experts checked the items, and necessary modifications were made. The test was also piloted among 15 students who did not take part in the study but were from the same population as that of the final sample. Based on the results, the Cronbach’s alpha index, as the measure of internal consistency, was 0.79, suggesting the reliability of the test as a whole.
One week prior to study proper, all the participants took the pretest. The study involved two treatment sessions, each lasting about two hours. During the sessions, the experimental group received the instructional multimedia lesson in which the isolated elements effect had been eliminated in order to reduce the extraneous cognitive load. The control group, in contrast, received the lesson with the isolated elements effect expected to induce a high level of extraneous cognitive load. At the end of the second session, the participants in both groups did the posttest. For analyzing the data, descriptive statistics followed by a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were applied. All the analyses were completed using SPSS (version 20).
Results
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics related to the participants’ scores on the English grammar test for both pretest and posttest.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics of Pretest and Posttest Scores across Groups in the Study (N = 36)
Measure |
Control group (n = 19) |
|
Experimental group (n = 17) |
||
M |
SD |
|
M |
SD |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Pretest |
3.47 |
2.81 |
|
3.63 |
2.69 |
Posttest |
9.94 |
2.22 |
|
13.53 |
2.09 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
As the table shows, the participants’ mean scores on the pretest for the two groups were close to one another before the treatment sessions. Nevertheless, the mean scores for both groups increased after the participants received their lessons with the mean score for the experimental group being higher than that of the other group. To investigate whether this difference on the posttest scores was statistically significant, an ANCOVA was conducted. The independent variable included group membership with two levels (viz., control and experimental), and the dependent variable was the scores on the posttest. The participants’ scores on the pretest were also added as the covariate to balance out their pre-existing differences in terms of the knowledge of the English grammar points delivered via the instructional multimedia lessons. Table 2 displays the results of the analysis.
Table 2. ANCOVA for Posttest Scores across Groups in the Study (N = 36)
Source |
Sum of squares |
df |
Mean squares |
F |
p |
Effect Sizea |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Pretest |
0.01 |
1 |
0.01 |
0.00 |
.951 |
.00 |
Group |
115.25 |
1 |
115.25 |
24.12 |
.000* |
.42 |
Error |
157.68 |
33 |
4.78 |
-- |
-- |
-- |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
* p< .0001.
a Partial eta-squared.
As viewed in the table, the participants in the experimental group (M=13.53, SD=2.09) significantly outperformed their counterparts in the control group (M=9.94, SD =2.22) on the posttest, F(1, 33) = 24.12, p = .000, partial eta-squared = .42. The effect size, which is large according to Cohen’s guidelines (19), showed that 42% of the variation in the posttest scores could be explained by group membership. On the whole, the results suggested that the instructional multimedia lesson designed based on minimizing the level of extraneous cognitive load had a positive effect on the learning of English grammar points among students of Nursing.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of instructional multimedia based on reducing the extraneous cognitive load in English language learning among Nursing students. The results showed that using instructional multimedia designed to reduce the extraneous cognitive load tends to improve students’ English language learning. This finding is consistent with results obtained in Khalil et al.’s study showing a decline in learning by increasing cognitive load caused by the complexity of instructional materials (10), Qiao et al.’s study reporting that due to high intrinsic cognitive load in courses such as anatomy, attempt should be made to reduce the extraneous cognitive load (17), Kahol et al.’s study indicating the impact of a simulator on improving scores based on the theory of cognitive load (11), Mosaramezani et al.’s study suggesting that reducing the extraneous cognitive load and managing intrinsic load at the same time could enhance students’ learning (14), and Zare et al.’s study supporting that using cognitive load theory strategies could help reduce the extraneous cognitive load and enhance learning in the physiology courses (18). According to the findings, instructional multimedia designed to reduce the extraneous cognitive load enhanced English language learning among Nursing students. This means that by reducing the extraneous cognitive load imposed on learners, their learning increases. Explanation of results is not unexpected, because no instruction without proper instructional design leads to effective learning. On the other hand, one of the requirements of instructional multimedia design is considering the cognitive load effects and applying them in the teaching-learning process. Cognitive load theory can be used in a wide range of learning environments, particularly instructional multimedia, because it relates the design of instructional materials to processing principles and cognitive load effects, and, as a result, learners’ cognitive load is minimized, and their learning is maximized. A limitation of this study includes the small sample size, making it difficult to generalize the results. According to the results of the study, it is suggested that educational specialists and multimedia designers should focus on different types of cognitive load principles and strategies to reduce and optimize cognitive load in instructional content.